Effects of nicotine administration via a sublingual tablet on arousal and verbal ability in non-smokers

نویسنده

  • J. Furedy
چکیده

Tobacco smoking or nicotine administration on its own can influence cognitive task performance. This study examined the effects of acute nicotine administration on verbal task performance and a physiological index of arousal. Healthy non-smoking participants received either a 2 mg sublingual nicotine tablet or placebo. Participants next completed various verbal tasks while heart rate recordings were taken concurrently. Nicotine increased heart rate relative to a pre-administration baseline period to indicate that the new method of nicotine administration increased cardiovascular arousal. Nicotine was associated with reduced accuracy in a verbal matching task and reduced accuracy and increased reaction time in an anagram task, but had no effect on performance during an analogy or verbal fluency task. The data suggest that nicotine naïve individuals are unlikely to gain any benefit to their verbal ability from the single acute intake of nicotine, such as that obtained from cigarette smoking or by using nicotine replacement therapy products. Nicotine and verbal ability 3 Introduction Nicotine activates nicotinic acetylcholine receptors that are located in many brain areas important for cognitive function (Levin, McClernon, & Rezvani, 2006). This may provide the psychopharmacological basis for why many smokers report that smoking improves functions such as attention and memory (Piper et al., 2004). However, somewhat surprisingly based on these reports, Newhouse, Potter, and Singh (2004) concluded in a review that there were an almost equal number of studies that showed improvements in cognition as there were studies that showed impairments (see also Heishman, Taylor, & Henningfield, 1994; Levin et al., 2006; Pogun 2001). For example, there are reports that nicotine enhances selective attention (e.g., Trimmel & Wittberger, 2004), sustained attention (Foulds et al., 1996; Levin et al., 1998), updating of working memory as reflected in P300 latency (Edwards, Wesnes, Warburton, & Gale, 1985; Houlihan, Pritchard, & Robinson, 1996), retrieval from working memory (Kerr, Sherwood, & Hindmarch, 1991), and recognition memory (Perkins et al., 1994). In contrast, there are reports that nicotine has no effect or impairs selective attention (Foulds et al., 1996; Heishman, Snyder, & Henningfield, 1993; Perkins et al., 1994), sustained attention (Wesnes, Warburton, & Matz, 1983), P300 latency (Ilan & Polich, 2001; Lindgren, Stenberg, & Rosen, 1998), retrieval from working memory (Foulds et al., 1996), and spatial working memory (Park, Knopick, McGurk, & Meltzer, 2000). These contradictory findings make it difficult to draw conclusions about the effects of nicotine on selective cognitive functions. The inconsistent findings may reflect any one of several factors. It has been argued that nicotine has a beneficial effect in smokers and individuals with neurological disorders and a detrimental effect in non-smokers (Newhouse et al., 2004). However, this is not always the case because there are reports in which nicotine impaired performance in smokers (e.g., Park et al., 1994) and improved performance in non-smokers (e.g., Trimmel & Wittberger, 2004). A more consistent finding is that the administration of nicotine reverses the negative impact of nicotine deprivation in smokers (Heishman et al., 1994), but from this it cannot be concluded that Nicotine and verbal ability 4 nicotine has a cognitive enhancing effect per se. To make this conclusion requires that studies utilise non-smokers who are free from the chronic effects of nicotine use. A potential difficulty in the use of non-smokers is that nicotine may have negative effects on mood and arousal (Kalman, 2002; Newhouse et al., 2004). Heishman and Henningfield (2000) provided a test of such claims when they administered nicotine to non-smokers across eight days. In spite of the participants developing tolerance to the negative subjective effects of nicotine, no performance improvements on a range of cognitive tasks were found. Research areas that contain contradictory findings will benefit from the accumulation of evidence from studies that employ a range of methods. The aggregated evidence will permit identification of patterns in the findings and the weight of evidence supporting a particular conclusion will become more apparent. The present study aimed to contribute to this gathering of knowledge by examining the effects of nicotine on tasks that have a verbal component. Nicotine administered to smokers has improved performance on a word recognition test (Foulds et al., 1996), a word matching task (Algan, Furedy, Demirgoren, Vincent, & Pogun, 1997), recall of words after a semantic processing task (Warburton, Skinner, & Martin, 2001), and recall of semantically related words (Rusted, Graupner, Tennant, & Warburton, 1998). The recall of words after five trials of learning a list of words (Min, Moon, Wo Ko, & Sang Shin, 2001) and the retrieval of words in a verbal fluency task (Howe & Price, 2001) is also enhanced by nicotine administration in elderly non-smokers at risk for dementia. However, no effect of nicotine was found with healthy non-smokers on a task that required the order of two pairs of letters to be determined (Ernst, Heishman, Spurgeon, & London, 2001), an anagram task (Marchant, Trawley, & Rusted, 2008; Rusted & Alvares, 2008), and an ongoing lexical decision task (Marchant et al., 2008). Also in non-smokers, nicotine has improved the retrievalinduced forgetting observed in episodic list learning (Rusted & Alvares, 2008) and performance in a lexical-semantic processing task (Holmes, Chenery, & Copland, 2008). Nicotine and verbal ability 5 In contrast to past research, the present experiment used a range of verbal tasks within one experiment. The tasks were selected to represent the components of verbal ability as defined by Campito (1994) as being composed of two broad facets: the amount and structure of verbal knowledge and the ability to reason with verbal information. A verbal fluency task (Halpern & Wright, 1996), anagram task (Walker, Liston, Hobson, & Stickgold, 2002), word matching task (Algan et al., 1997), and an analogy task (Gitomer, Glaser, Curtis, & Lensky, 1982) were used. The first three tasks are most closely linked to verbal knowledge and the last task is related to the ability to reason with verbal information. While these tasks will involve other aspects of cognitive function, such as attention and memory, they are ecologically valid with respect to these verbal components. This is particularly relevant given that smoking often occurs in situations involving interpersonal communication (e.g., social gatherings and work breaks) and many work tasks use printed and spoken words. The present experiment also tested a new method of nicotine administration. The typical use of transdermal nicotine skin patches (e.g., Howe & Price 2001; Min et al., 2001) requires a long absorption period of two hours or more to reach peak levels of nicotine. Absorption of nicotine through the oral mucosa is faster than absorption through the external skin (Molander & Lunell 2001). The present experiment thus employed a sublingual tablet that contained 2 mg of nicotine bound to β-cyclodextrin. Peak levels of nicotine are reached at 1 hour and do not substantially decline until after 2 hours following administration (Molander & Lunell, 2001). A measure of cardiovascular arousal (heart rate; HR) was taken independent from and during the verbal tasks to document the physiological effect of the nicotine tablet. Furthermore, nonsmoking participants were tested to avoid the potentially confounding effects of chronic nicotine use in smoking participants. Method Participants Thirty-nine female and 43 male first year psychology students aged between 18 and 29 Nicotine and verbal ability 6 years (M = 19.0, SD = 2.00) participated in exchange for course credit. All except one participant reported to have never smoked. The remaining participant had quit smoking five years previously. Inclusion or exclusion of this participant did not affect the outcome of the statistical analyses and the participant was retained. No participants reported use of any nicotine replacement therapy product. All participants were required to have English as their primary language and reported that they did not have a history of hypertension, cardiac disease, cerebrovascular disease, impaired renal function, pregnancy, seizure, abuse or dependence on alcohol or other drugs, psychiatric illness, and current use of any medications. Blood pressure and HR was also measured. Seven males and one female were excluded due to high blood pressure (>140/90) and two females were excluded due to a non-English speaking background. The final sample consisted of 72 participants (36 male, 36 female) wherein half of each sex were randomly allocated to receive nicotine or placebo. The demographic characteristics of each group are shown in Table 1. All groups were similar in age, weight, prior years of education, heart rate, as well as diastolic and systolic blood pressure assessed during screening, all ts < 0.85, p > .05. All participants provided informed consent to a protocol approved by the Griffith University Human Research Ethics Committee. ------------------------------Insert Table 1 about here -----------------------------Apparatus Nicotine administration. Participants were given either a sublingual Nicorette Microtab containing 2 mg of nicotine or a placebo that was matched as closely as possible to the nicotine tablet in size, color, and taste. To negate the sensory properties of the tablets (e.g., size, taste), they were placed under each participant’s tongue to dissolve using a blind protocol. Pilot research indicated that participants are unaware of the nature of the tablet using this administration method. Participants were instructed not to chew, not to move the tablet around, Nicotine and verbal ability 7 and not to swallow more than once per minute (a timer was used to assist this requirement). The participants watched the same video program for 45 min and were subsequently escorted to a testing room for set up of the HR recordings and instructions for the experimental tasks. Testing began 1 hour after administration. Prior research has indicated that nicotine plasma concentrations reach a peak mean level of 3.8 ± ng/ml (SD = 1.0) at 1 hour following administration of a single 2 mg microtab (Molander & Lunell, 2001). The presence of adverse reactions to the nicotine tablet and placebo was monitored through observation and questioning by an experimenter. The most common side effects reported by participants were a dry mouth and an increase in coughing. One participant that was administered nicotine reported feelings of dizziness. This participant (female), however, was not sufficiently unwell to discontinue the experiment; she completed the entire experimental protocol. An examination of her data indicated that she was not deviant from the other females in her group, exclusion of her data did not change the pattern of results, and her data was thus retained in the analyses. Physiological response measurement. Recordings were obtained via a PowerLab Model 4/20 data acquisition system (ADInstruments, Sydney). Heart rate was measured by an ADInstruments Model MLT1010 Piezo Electric Pulse transducer attached to the distal phalange of the third finger of the non-preferred hand. Responses were sampled at 1000 Hz starting with the onset of the task or baseline period and terminated at the end of the task or the baseline period. Verbal tasks. Three of the verbal tasks (word matching, analogy, and anagram) were presented on Dell Optiplex Model GX240 and GX1 computers. The word matching task (Algan et al., 1997) consisted of 16 practice trials and 112 test trials. On each trial, participants were presented with a fixation cross for 1000 ms, followed by a visual display for 600 ms. The display consisted of two letter pairs above and to the left and right of the fixation cross and one letter pair below and to either the left or right of the fixation cross. A match occurred when one of the letter pairs above the fixation cross (e.g., BA) formed an English word when combined Nicotine and verbal ability 8 with the letter pair below (e.g., NK). A mismatch occurred when a word could not be formed. An equal number of trials were match and mismatch trials. To manipulate difficulty, 192 high frequency four letter words (M = 1117.29 times per million) and 192 low frequency four letter words (M = 9.45 times per million) were selected (see Leech, Rayson, & Wilson, 2001). Participants were asked to answer as quickly and accurately as possible by pressing either the letter “B” or “N” (counterbalanced) for a match or mismatch response on the keyboard. The dependent measures were correct RT and proportion of correct responses. The analogy task used six practice trials and 32 test trials. In each trial, participants were presented with an analogy stem (e.g., car : road :: train : ) for 5 s, followed by four possible answers (e.g., (a) wheel (b) vehicle (c) fast (d) track ) for a maximum of 15 s. Participants were required to infer a relationship between the first two words in the analogy stem, and transfer that relationship to the third word and one of the answers. Participants indicated their answer on the keyboard and were asked to answer as accurately as possible. Difficulty was varied by categorizing the word answer for half t as a high frequency (M = 74.12 per million) and half as low frequency (M = 5.43 per million) (Leech et al., 2001). The dependent measures were the proportion of correct answers and RT to enter the answer. The anagram task used six practice trials and 32 test trials (Rajaram & Roediger, 1993). Participants were presented with a scrambled word for a maximum of 20 s and were to unscramble the letters to form a concrete word (e.g., ETNPAU = PEANUT). Participants were required to answer as quickly and accurately as possible by typing their answer on the keyboard. To manipulate difficulty, half of the words comprised five to six letters (short words) and the remaining comprised seven to eight letters (long words). The dependent variables were the proportion of correct responses and the RT to enter the first letter of the answer. In the verbal fluency task participants were presented with four letters (F, A, S and N), each separately on paper. Starting with the letter F, participants were asked to generate as many words as possible that begin with the letter for 60 s. The procedure continued for the remaining Nicotine and verbal ability 9 letters of A, S, and N. Participants were required to write the words down, rather than say them out aloud (cf. Halpern & Wright, 1996), because another participant in the experiment may have been completing the tablet administration in an adjacent room. Participants could not derive words from those they had already written (e.g., play, to form plays, played and playing). The total number of unique English words produced was counted. Procedure After consent and screening, participants rinsed their mouth with water and were directed to the testing room for attachment of the electrodes for HR recordings. Participants sat quietly for 3 min during which time pre-administration recordings for HR were taken. The nicotine or matched placebo tablet was next administered. After the absorption period, the tasks were administered. The experimenter gave the instructions for the word matching task and participants completed the practice trials. After any questions were answered, the participant completed the main experimental trials, while HR was recorded continuously. A similar procedure followed for the analogy task, anagram task, and verbal fluency task. At the end of the verbal fluency task, participants sat for another 3 min while no task post-administration recordings were taken. Participants were debriefed at the end of the experiment. Data reduction Heart rate was taken as the mean value across the entire duration of each verbal task and the no task baseline periods. The mean proportion correct for the word matching task, analogy task, and anagram task were calculated across all trials. Non-responses were counted as incorrect. The RTs for these three tasks were also calculated using only correct responses. Preliminary analyses indicated that the within-subjects manipulations of task difficulty did not interact with either nicotine or sex. For this reason, the performance data were collapsed across all trials and statistical analyses employed a 2 x 2 (Nicotine x Sex) factorial design and assessed against a two-tailed α = .05. Effect sizes are reported as Cohen’s d. Results Nicotine and verbal ability 10 Heart rate measures To take into account individual differences in baseline levels, HR obtained during the pre-administration baseline period was subtracted from that obtained during the verbal tasks and the no-task baseline period following administration. A positive change indicates that HR was higher following the administration of nicotine or placebo than during the preadministration period. As shown in Figure 1, HR tended to be higher in participants given nicotine than in participants given placebo. A series of 2 x 2 (Nicotine x Sex) ANOVAs confirmed that the difference was significant during the no-task baseline period, F (1, 67) = 9.37, d = .74, p < .05, and during all verbal tasks, all Fs > 5.53, d > .56, p < .05. No other main effects or interactions were found, all Fs < 1.64, p > .05. ------------------------------Insert Figure 1 about here ------------------------------Performance measures Performance on each verbal task was examined with separate 2 x 2 (Nicotine x Sex) ANOVAs. Table 2 shows the mean values for each performance measure in each task. In no cases was there a significant Nicotine x Sex interaction, all Fs < 3.94, p > .05. However, as can be seen, the nicotine and placebo groups did differ in performance for some tasks, but not others. Participants administered nicotine showed significantly lower accuracy in the word matching task, F (1, 68) = 4.10, d = .56, p < .05, significantly lower accuracy in the anagram task, F (1, 68) = 5.08, d = .54, p < .05, and significantly longer RT in the anagram task, F (1, 68) = 8.64, d = .70, p < .05, than participants administered placebo. Performance did not differ significantly between the nicotine and placebo administration conditions for the analogy task or the verbal fluency task, all Fs < 3.21, d < .42, p > .05. ------------------------------Insert Table 2 about here Nicotine and verbal ability 11 -----------------------------A number of sex differences also emerged with the tasks. Females (M = 983 ms, SD = 333) had a shorter RT than males (M = 1226 ms, SD = 427) for the word matching task, F (1, 68) = 7.18, d = .63, p < .05. Accuracy on the word matching task was higher in males (M = 0.72, SD = 0.10) than in females (M = 0.65, SD = 0.07), F (1, 68) = 9.35, d = .81, p < .05. Finally, the expected sex difference in the analogy task of higher accuracy for males (M = 0.55, SD = 0.11) than females (M = 0.50, SD = 0.11) was present, F (1, 68) = 4.63, d = .64, p < .05. The differences between males and females for the anagram task or the verbal fluency task were not significant, all Fs < 2.88, p > .05. Discussion The administration method that was used, the application of a sublingual tablet containing 2 mg of nicotine, increased HR during the verbal tasks and during a no task baseline period. The observation that nicotine increases HR is consistent with prior reports (e.g., Foulds et al., 1997; Furedy, Algan, Vincent, Demirgoren, & Pogun, 1999). The mean size of the HR increase across all measurement periods was 3.12 bpm (SD = 1.59). Foulds et al. reported an increase of 2.8 bpm following a 0.3 mg injection of nicotine in non-smokers. One study that administered a 7 mg transdermal nicotine patch to smokers and non-smokers showed a transient increase in HR of 3.14 bpm between 180 and 240 minutes following administration (Poltavski & Petros, 2005). It may be concluded that the effect of the 2 mg sublingual tablet on increasing cardiovascular arousal is comparable to that obtained in some other studies that administer nicotine via other means and supports the utility of sublingual tablets as a nicotine administration method. The use of nicotine tablets affords one advantage in that it provides a non-invasive means to deliver a dose of nicotine within a convenient timeframe for testing. It also avoids variation across participants caused by differences in the speed and intensity of chewing movements when nicotine gum is used to administer nicotine orally. Nicotine and verbal ability 12 Nicotine administration influenced verbal task performance for some tasks, but not others, and it was not influenced by sex or task difficulty. The null effects of nicotine in solving verbal analogy problems replicated the findings reported by Marchant et al. (2008) and Rusted and Alvares (2008). Nicotine did have an effect on performance in the word matching and anagram task. In both cases, nicotine was associated with poorer performance on these tasks. The tasks seem to depend on verbal knowledge (Campito, 1994), specifically lexical knowledge for the word matching task (Brand, Bekhum, Stumpel, & Kroeze, 1983) and lexical knowledge in addition to verbal flexibility for the anagram task (Walker et al., 2002). The tasks also depend on non-verbal cognitive functions such as sustained attention and memory retrieval and these factors may also be implicated in the impairments found. The impairments in performance for the word matching and anagram tasks contribute to the accumulating evidence that nicotine can have no effect or impair performance on tasks that have a verbal component (Ernst et al., 2001; Marchant et al., 2008; Rusted & Alvares, 2008) or on tasks that assess other cognitive functions (e.g., Foulds et al., 1996; Heishman et al., 1993; Ilan & Polich, 2001; Lindgren et al., 1998; Park et al., 2000; Perkins et al., 1994). Performance on cognitive tasks may also be mediated by nicotine-induced changes in other states. For instance, changes in mood (see review by Kalman, 2002; Kalman & Smith, 2005) or physical state induced by nicotine may have been an important factor. The low dosage (2 mg) nicotine tablet was selected in order to reduce the potential for adverse reactions from the participants. Moreover, participants were carefully monitored and questioned by the experimenter and only one participant reported significant side effects (dizziness). No participant withdrew from the study from adverse reactions. This suggests that the effects of nicotine on changing mood or producing side-effects in the present study were, if present, minor. The study by Heishman and Henningfield (2000) also suggested that negative subjective effects may not always explain a lack of performance improvement following nicotine administration. In addition, nicotine did not impair performance on all of the tasks used. The Nicotine and verbal ability 13 selective effect of nicotine on specific tasks suggests that global mood or physical states may not have caused the performance decrement. However, this argument is tentative because inspection of the mean values for the analogy and verbal fluency tasks indicated that the differences between groups, though nonsignificant, were in a direction consistent with poorer performance following nicotine administration. The argument would be stronger if the differences between groups were reversed in these tasks or if there was an interaction in which the anagram and word matching tasks showed significantly larger impairments by nicotine than the analogy and verbal fluency tasks. Further research should aim to provide measures of mood and side-effect states (e.g., Foulds et al., 1997). Appropriate statistical analyses could thus determine if any mood changes that are found mediate the effects of nicotine on verbal ability or merely represent an independent effect of nicotine administration. Males and females have been shown to differ in performance during some tasks that assess verbal ability. Prior research has shown better performance in females than in males on the verbal fluency, anagram, and word matching tasks, whereas the opposite results are seen with the analogy task (Algan et al., 1997; Halpern, 2000; Hyde & Linn, 1988). In the present study, females performed better than males (shorter RT) in the word matching task, whereas males performed better (higher accuracy) in the word matching task. The sex differences observed for the word matching task thus appear to reflect a speed-accuracy tradeoff in that females, relative to males, responded faster at a cost of reduced accuracy. The expected sex difference of higher accuracy in males than females in the analogy task was also found. However, the effects of nicotine did not interact with sex for any tasks to suggest that the effects of nicotine were independent of any sex differences in task performance. In conclusion, the present results indicate that acute nicotine administration impairs performance on verbal tasks linked to verbal flexibility and lexical knowledge. While the use of non-smoking participants in the present study may limit generalizations that can be made to a smoking population, this methodology allowed the effects of nicotine administration to be Nicotine and verbal ability 14 examined independent of chronic smoking status and withdrawal effects present in smokers. Additional research is required that uses the present tasks, which are more ecologically valid than many verbal tasks used in previous research, to determine the performance effects in smokers. Nevertheless, nicotine naïve individuals would appear unlikely to gain any benefits in verbal performance from the single acute intake of nicotine, such as that obtained after smoking a cigarette or the use of nicotine replacement therapy products. Such outcomes should act as a deterrent to individuals who are contemplating the use of tobacco or nicotine products because of any putative enhancement of verbal function. Nicotine and verbal ability 15 Acknowledgements The present research was supported by Research Grant CPY GURDG to the first author. Nicotine and verbal ability 16 ReferencesAlgan, O., Furedy, J. J., Demirgoren, S., Vincent, A., & Pogun, S. (1997). Effects of tobaccosmoking and gender on interhemispheric cognitive function: performance andconfidence measures. Behavioural Pharmacology, 8, 416-428.Brand, N., Bekkum, I. V., Stumpel, M., & Kroeze, J. H. (1983). Word Matching and LexicalDecisions: A Visual Half-Field Study. Brain and Language, 18, 199-211.Campito, J. S. (1994). Verbal ability. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Encyclopedia of HumanIntelligence (pp. 1106-1115). New York: Macmillan.Edwards, J. A., Wesnes, K., Warburton, D. M., & Gale, A. (1985). Evidence of more rapidstimulus evaluation following cigarette smoking. Addictive Behaviors, 10, 113–126.Ernst, M., Heishman, S. J., Spurgeon, L., & London, E. D. (2001). Smoking History andNicotine Effects on Cognitive Performance. Neuropsychopharmacology, 25, 313-319.Foulds, J., Stapleton, J. A., Bell, N., Swettenham, J., Jarvis, M. J., & Russell, M. A. H. (1997).Mood and physiological effects of subcutaneous nicotine in smokers and never-smokers. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 44, 105-115.Foulds, J., Stapleton, J., Swettenham, J., Bell, N., McSorely, K., & Russell, M. A. H. (1996). Cognitive performance effects of subcutaneous nicotine in smokers and never-smokers.Psychopharmacology, 127, 31-38.Furedy, J. J., Algan, O., Vincent, A., Demirgoren, S., & Pogun, S. (1999). Sexually dimorphiceffect of an acute smoking manipulation on skin resistance but not on heart-rate duringa cognitive verbal task, Integrative Physiological & BehaviouralScience, 34, 219-226. Gitomer, D. H., Glaser, R. G., Curtis, M. E., & Lensky, D. B. (1982). Processing Differencesas a Function of Item Difficulty in Verbal Analogy Performance. Journal ofEducational Psychology, 79, 212-219.Halpern, D. F. (2000). Sex Differences in Cognitive Abilities (3rd ed.). New Jersey: LawrenceErlbaum Associates. Nicotine and verbal ability 17 Halpern, D. F., & Wright, T. M. (1996). A process-oriented model of cognitive sex differences.Learning and Individual Differences, 8, 3-24.Heishman, S. J., & Henningfield, J. E. (2000). Tolerance to repeated nicotine administration onperformance, subjective, and physiological responses in nonsmokers.Psychopharmacology, 152, 321-333.Heishman, S. J., Snyder, F. R., & Henningfield, J. E. (1993). Performance, subjective, andphysiological effects of nicotine in non-smokers. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 34,11-18.Heishman, S. J., Taylor, R. C., & Henningfield, J. E. (1994). Nicotine and smoking: A reviewof effects on human performance. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 2,345-395.Holmes, A. D., Chenery, H. J., & Copland, D. A. (2008). Transdermal nicotine modulatesstrategy-based attentional semantic processing in non-smokers. International Journal ofNeuropsychopharmacology, 11, 389-399.Houlihan, M. E., Pritchard, W. S., & Robinson, J. H. (1996). Faster P300 latency after smokingin visual but not auditory oddball tasks. Psychopharmacology, 123, 231–238. Howe, M. N., & Price, I. R. (2001). Effects of Transdermal Nicotine on Learning, Memory, Verbal Fluency, Concentration, and General Health in a Healthy Sample at Risk forDementia. International Psychogeriatrics, 13, 465-475.Hyde, J. S., & Linn, M. C. (1988). Gender Differences in Verbal Ability: A Metaanalysis.Psychological Bulletin, 104, 53-69. Ilan, A. B., & Polich, J. (2001). Tobacco smoking and event-related brain potentials in a Strooptask. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 40, 109-118.Kalman, D. (2002). The subjective effects of nicotine: methodological issues, a review ofexperimental studies, and recommendations for future research. Nicotine & TobaccoResearch, 4, 25-70. Nicotine and verbal ability 18 Kalman, D., & Smith, S. S. (2005). Does nicotine do what we think it does? A meta-analyticreview of the subjective effects of nicotine in nasal spray and intravenous studies withsmokers and nonsmokers. Nicotine and Tobacco Research, 7, 317-333.Kerr, J. S., Sherwood, N., & Hindmarch, I. (1991). Separate and combined effects of the socialdrugs on psychomotor performance. Psychopharmacology, 104, 113–119.Leech, G., Rayson, P., Wilson, A. (2001). Word frequencies in written and spoken English:Based on the British National Corpus. Essex, UK: Longman.Levin, E. D., Conners, C. K., Silva, D., Hinton, S. C., Meck, W. H., March, J., & Rose, J. E.(1998). Transdermal nicotine effects on attention. Psychopharmacology, 140, 135–141.Levin, E. D., McClernon, J. F., & Rezvani, A. H. (2006). Nicotinic effects on cognitivefunction: behavioral characterization, pharmacological specification, and anatomiclocalization. Psychopharmacology, 184, 523-539.Lindgren, M., Stenberg, G., & Rosen, I. (1998). Effects of nicotine in a bimodal attention task.Neuropsychobiology, 38, 42–49.Marchant, N. L., Trawley, S., & Rusted, J. M. (2008). Prospective memory or prospectiveattention: physiological and pharmacological support for an attention model.International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology, 11, 401-411. Min, S. K., Moon, I. W., Ko, R. W., & Shin, H. S. (2001). Effects of transdermal nicotine onattention and memory in healthy elderly non-smokers. Psychopharmacology, 159, 83-88. Molander, L., & Lunell, E. (2001). Pharmacokinetic investigation of a nicotine sublingualtablet. European Journal of Clinical Pharmacology, 56, 813-819.Newhouse, P. A., Potter, A., & Singh, A. (2004). Effects of nicotinic stimulation on cognitiveperformance. Current Opinion in Pharmacology, 4, 36-46. Park, S., Knopick, C., McGurk, S., & Meltzer, H. Y. (2000). Nicotine impairs spatial workingmemory while leaving spatial attention intact. Neuropsychopharmacology, 22, 200-209. Nicotine and verbal ability 19 Perkins, K. A., Grobe, J. E., Fonte, C., Goettler, J., Caggiula, A. R., Reynolds, W. A., Stiller, R.L., Scierka, A., & Jacob, R. G. (1994). Chronic and acute tolerance to subjective,behavioral and cardiovascular effects of nicotine in humans. Journal of Pharmacologyand Experimental Therapeutics, 270, 628–638.Piper, M. E., Piasecki, T. M., Federman, E. B., Bolt, D. M., Smith, S. S., Fiore, M. C., & Baker,T. B. (2004). A multiple motives approach to tobacco dependence: The Wisconsininventory of smoking dependence motives (WISDM-68). Journal of Consulting andClinical Psychology, 72, 139-154.Pogun, S. (2001). Sex differences in brain and behavior: emphasis on nicotine, nitric oxide andplace learning.International Journal of Psychophysiology, 4, 195-208.Poltavski, D. V., & Petros, T. (2005). Effects of transdermal nicotine on prose memory andattention in smokers and nonsmokers. Physiology & Behavior, 83, 833-843.Rajaram, S., & Roediger, H. L. (1993). Direct Comparison of Four Implicit Memory Tests.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 19, 765-776.Rusted, J. M., & Alvares, T. (2008). Nicotine effects on retrieval-induced forgetting are notattributable to changes in arousal. Psychopharmacology, 196, 83-92. Rusted, J. M., Graupner, L., Tennant, A., & Warburton, D. M. (1998). Effortful processing is arequirement for nicotine-induced improvements in memory. Psychopharmacology, 138,362-368. Trimmel, M., & Wittberger, S. (2004). Effects of transdermally administered nicotine onaspects of attention, task load, and mood in women and men. Pharmacology,Biochemistry and Behavior, 78, 639-645.Walker, M. P., Liston, C., Hobson, A., & Stickgold, R. (2002). Cognitive flexibility across thesleep-wake cycle: REM-sleep enhancement of anagram problem solving. CognitiveBrain Research, 14, 317-324. Nicotine and verbal ability 20 Warburton, D. M., Skinner, A., & Martin, C. D. (2001). Improved incidental memory withnicotine after semantic processing, but not after phonological processing.Psychopharmacology, 153, 258-263.Wesnes, K., Warburton, D. M., & Matz, B. (1983). Effects of nicotine on stimulus sensitivityand response bias in a visual vigilance task. Neuropsychobiology, 9, 41–44. Nicotine and verbal ability 21 Table 1Mean values for the demographic characteristics for the participants administered nicotine (n= 36) and placebo (n = 36)(standard deviations are in parentheses). MeasureAdministration condition

برای دانلود متن کامل این مقاله و بیش از 32 میلیون مقاله دیگر ابتدا ثبت نام کنید

ثبت نام

اگر عضو سایت هستید لطفا وارد حساب کاربری خود شوید

منابع مشابه

Local tolerance of a sublingual nicotine tablet, an open single-centre study.

Nicotine replacement therapy (NRT) is now widely used in various forms of administration to aid cessation of tobacco use. In this smoking cessation programme, a new tablet formulation has been compared to and found bioequivalent to the existing one. The present trial was performed in order to investigate the local tolerance of the new sublingual tablet. The study was performed as a prospective ...

متن کامل

Nesbitt's Paradox resolved? Stress and arousal modulation during cigarette smoking.

Nesbitt's Paradox states that cigarette smoking generates physiological and psychological changes which are normally incompatible, namely increased arousal together with decreased stress. This review confirms these changes, but shows that they are dependent upon various factors, particularly the degree of nicotine deprivation. Thus the relaxant properties of smoking reflect the relief of irrita...

متن کامل

Are the inhibitory effects of nicotine on erectile response in nonsmokers generalizable to long-term smokers? A reply.

Thank you for bringing to our attention your study examining the effects of smoking cessation on erectile functioning in men. Guay and colleagues [1] found no difference in erectile response during nicotine administration (via a 21-mg transdermal patch) compared to no nicotine administration (24 hours postcessation), while Harte and Meston [2] demonstrated that nonsmoking men exhibited signific...

متن کامل

Biotechnological Reduction of Tobacco (Nicotiana Tabacum L.) Toxicity

BACKGROUND: Nicotiana tobacco contains large amounts of alkaloid nicotine. Tobacco plant is used for smoking and causes many health problems since it is high in nicotine which is one of the widely-recognized toxic compounds with serious side effects for different body organs. Reducing nicotine content of this plant is a way to reduce its health hazards in cigarette smokers. Utilizing the new ...

متن کامل

Acute cardiovascular and sympathetic effects of nicotine replacement therapy.

Sympathetic overactivity is implicated in the increased cardiovascular risk of cigarette smokers. Excitatory nicotinic receptors are present on peripheral chemoreceptor cells. Chemoreceptors located in the carotid and aortic bodies increase ventilation (Ve), blood pressure (BP), heart rate (HR), and sympathetic nerve activity to muscle circulation (MSNA) in response to hypoxia. We tested the hy...

متن کامل

ذخیره در منابع من


  با ذخیره ی این منبع در منابع من، دسترسی به آن را برای استفاده های بعدی آسان تر کنید

برای دانلود متن کامل این مقاله و بیش از 32 میلیون مقاله دیگر ابتدا ثبت نام کنید

ثبت نام

اگر عضو سایت هستید لطفا وارد حساب کاربری خود شوید

عنوان ژورنال:

دوره   شماره 

صفحات  -

تاریخ انتشار 2017